How Horses Feel and Think - Marlitt Wendt - E-Book

How Horses Feel and Think E-Book

Marlitt Wendt

0,0

Beschreibung

Take a glimpse of the world seen from a horse's point of view This is a fascinating journey into the emotional world seen from a horse's point of view. Thoroughly researched and presented, this book introduces the reader to the language of horses, to their astonishing mental capabilities and their deepest emotions. The information provided offers a good basis for horse owners to learn how to relate better to their horses, to develop a more harmonious relationship to their horses and to school their horses without using force but in a positive, pro-active way. Written in an accessible, sometimes light-hearted way from a practical point of view, the author succeeds in bridging the gap between science and the daily experience of everybody dealing with horses.

Sie lesen das E-Book in den Legimi-Apps auf:

Android
iOS
von Legimi
zertifizierten E-Readern

Seitenzahl: 126

Das E-Book (TTS) können Sie hören im Abo „Legimi Premium” in Legimi-Apps auf:

Android
iOS
Bewertungen
0,0
0
0
0
0
0
Mehr Informationen
Mehr Informationen
Legimi prüft nicht, ob Rezensionen von Nutzern stammen, die den betreffenden Titel tatsächlich gekauft oder gelesen/gehört haben. Wir entfernen aber gefälschte Rezensionen.



 

Marlitt Wendt

 

How Horses Feel and Think

 

 

 

Understanding Behaviour, Emotions and Intelligence

Imprint

Copyright © 2011 Cadmos Publishing Ltd, Richmond, UK

Copyright of original edition © 2009 Cadmos Verlag GmbH, Schwarzenbek, Germany

Design of print edition: Ravenstein + Partner, Verden Setting: Das Agenturhaus, Munich

Cover photograph: Christiane Slawik

Content photos: Christiane Slawik

Drawings: Maria Mähler

Translation: Dr Thomas Ritter

Editorial of the original edition: Anneke Bosse

Editorial of this edition: Christopher Long

E-Book: Satzweiss.com Print Web Software GmbH

All rights reserved: No Part of this book may be reprinted or reproduced or utilized in any form or by any electronic, mechanical, or other means, now known or hereafter invented, including photocopying and recording, or in any information storage or retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publisher.

British Library Cataloguing in Publication Data

A catalogue record of this book is available from the British Library.

ISBN 978-0-85788-000-0

eISBN 978-0-85788-608-8

A Journey of Discovery into Equine Psychology

A Journey of Discovery into Equine Psychology

Have you ever wondered what it would be like to see the world through the eyes of another person, just for a day? How do they experience their emotions? Do they see colours like I do? How does their smile feel? Well, what would a day in the emotional life of a horse be like? An exciting thought experiment!

Of course there are limits to how far you can explore the experiential world of another being, especially of another species. Yet, by now, so many insights have been gained into the structure of the horse’s brain, into its hormonal regulatory system, into learning behaviour and many other areas of equine life that had previously been inaccessible to us, that we can at least get a glimpse of this strange world. As our knowledge of science and animal physiology develops, it is becoming clear that the brains of all mammals resemble each other a great deal with respect to their basic structure and functions. Differences between the equine brain and the human brain certainly do exist, but they are a matter of degree rather than of basic principle. However, we should not make the mistake of humanising horses, but instead must accept their unique character traits and emotional idiosyncrasies as the heritage of their wild ancestors.

In addition, every horse has its own personality as a wonderful creature with its own unique world of experience. We will see how multi-faceted its thoughts and emotions have to be. The explanatory models of the traditional riding manuals regarding behaviour are not even remotely adequate to do justice to the nature of the horse.

As a horse lover and behavioural biologist, I want to use this book to build a bridge between the world of scientific research and the equestrian world, using today’s scientific insight to present the natural rights and needs of the horse. Current research findings and explanatory models from behavioural biology help us to understand many interesting phenomena of equine life. I want to let you participate in the variety of studies and the thoughts of other researchers, and to give you a first insight into equine psychology.

But there is infinitely more to learn here. Therefore, embark on a journey of discovery! Let yourself be inspired by the texts, examine the photos closely and gather as much information as possible. Beyond this, there is only one being that can help you to get to know the personality of the horses: the horse itself!

Let us now delve into the fascinating world of horses – perhaps we will see it with somewhat different eyes afterwards.

 

Marlitt Wendt,

February 2009

 

The Ethology and Evolution of the Horse

The Ethology and Evolution of the Horse

B

efore addressing individual equine learning and living experiences, I want to give an overview of the nature of the horse and the methods of behavioural biology. Horses behave more or less the same way today as their ancestors have always done, in the ways which have proved most useful in nature. Even domestication by man has not been able to change this. Ethology, the science of behaviour, can help us understand the horse’s needs better and to participate in its experiential world. For this purpose we have to consider our means of influencing the horse, as well as its natural potential: Every horse is a unique individual, a product of its genetic heritage as well as its environment. Horses are born with certain behaviours, while others are acquired through life experiences. Behavioural science sums this construct up in the term ‘nature and nurture’, making it clear that both areas have a significant influence on the horse’s personality.

What is Behaviour?

At first sight, the question might seem trivial – however, ‘behaviour’ is a central term and actually quite difficult to define, as it has many levels of interpretation. A grazing horse is exhibiting a behavioural pattern every bit as complex as a horse that is galloping, playing, or doing the piaffe. Such activities are always compounds of a variety of interacting mechanisms, and when evaluating a behaviour, all observable physical activities should be taken into consideration. Depending on the complexity of the behaviour, there may be an incredibly large number of physical features and changes. Take the example of the walk, for instance: how are the individual limbs moving, precisely? What is the rest of the body doing? Have you really considered every part of the body, all the muscles, and the surface of the skin? What breathing frequency can be observed? What is the expression of the eyes? In addition, we can describe the presumed purpose of the horse’s actions. Where is it going? What does its facial expression say? Many interpretive elements are entering the equation here. It is the question ‘Why?’ that especially represents one of the central issues in behavioural science, and it can be answered on many very different levels.

 

When we observe horses in a pasture, we can study the range of their possible behaviours – all we have to do is pay close attention.

 

Tinbergen’s Questions

Nikolaas Tinbergen is considered one of the foremost behavioural scientists of the 20th century. Together with Konrad Lorenz and Karl von Frisch, he was awarded the Nobel Prize for Medicine in 1973. Tinbergen developed the dominant theory of modern behavioural biology, which became known as ‘„Tinbergen’s Questions’, and which deals with the causes, functions, individual development, as well as the evolutionary development of behaviour. According to this elementary theory, there are four essentially equally important levels of answering any question we can ask about a horse’s behaviour: causality, functionality, individual development and collective evolutionary development.

Let’s take the example of a spooky horse, and answer Tinbergen’s questions:

 

• The question of causality. You can initially answer this question on the level of the direct, immediate cause. A horse spooks because it has sensitive sensory organs that can send nervous impulses to the brain very quickly and lead to an immediate reaction.

• The question of functionality. Spooking gives our horse an acute survival advantage. It is able to react immediately to potential dangers and to protect its own life through flight.

• Spooking therefore fulfils a function that is necessary for survival in the horse’s natural environment.

• The question of the individual development. Every horse will have observed the important ability to spook in its own individual developmental history, its life story, by observing its mother and other herd members, and will have learned this behaviour to an individually varying degree.

• The question of the collective evolutionary development. Spooking has furthermore proven to be an elementary behaviour in the horse’s evolutionary development. For millions of years, the ancestors of our modern horses had to evade dangerous predators. Our modern horses are thus the direct descendants of extremely spooky quadrupeds that were able to ensure their survival as a species based on this behavioural structure. We should always keep this in the back of our mind when we complain about the excessive spookiness of our horses, or when we make fun of it.

 

Instincts in Horses

You could translate the term ‘instinct’ literally as ‘natural urge’. An instinct refers to the inner, unknown impulses behind an animal’s behaviour that an observer can witness. Colloquially, we call behaviour ‘instinctive’ when it happens spontaneously, ‘from the gut’, without conscious deliberation. For many years, behavioural science was based on what is called instinct theory: an instinctual movement was supposed to be the result of an animal’s spontaneously arising inner readiness to act, which is triggered by a key stimulus once it reaches a specific stimulation threshold. In the widest sense, we understand instinctive behaviour to mean the horse’s typical, innate behaviour. By today’s scientific standards this view is outdated, as these simple basic assumptions cannot stand up to the new neurobiological explanatory models. A horse acts in much more complex ways and has to be considered a personality, not an ‘instinct machine’. The assumption that horses always act according to a simple stimulusreaction principle neglects the fact that every behaviour consists of the interaction between emotional states, previous individual experiences, and conscious thought processes, as well as the individual, highly specific situation.

 

Horses are not just ‘instinct machines’ – their actions are the results of complex interactions between emotions, experiences, and thought processes.

 

Ethology and Psychology Go Hand in Hand

Until the 1960s, only the ‘nature’ aspect of the ‘nature and nurture’ combination was considered relevant to classical horse researchers. They predominantly studied horses in their natural environment and presented their arguments mainly from the point of view of evolutionary history. It was only with the incorporation of the innovative approach of psychology, which focuses on the ‘nurture’ aspect by researching the development of individual behaviour and the learning processes, that a new dynamic entered equine ethology.

In modern behavioural biology these two explanatory models are now inseparably interwoven, in order to be able to grasp the horse’s behavioural repertoire in its totality. Every sentient being’s personality represents the sum of innate and learned elements, which interact constantly. For instance, the innate behaviour of a foal triggers a certain behavioural response from the mother, from which the foal in turn learns something which informs his future behaviour.

Modern equine researchers speak of a model of ‘nature via nurture’. There are genetic traits that are switched on only after the individual has had the appropriate experiences. Both horses and humans, for example, are born with the ability to see, being equipped with eyes and the necessary nervous system. But if the exterior stimuli were missing, if we grew up in complete darkness, our vision would never develop. We would be functionally blind, although born with all the physical prerequisites for eyesight.

Modern equine research studies the inter action of innate and learned behaviours. This requires the statistical evaluation of as much comparable data as possible. An isolated observation by a horse-owner cannot produce a generalised statement about equine behaviour – no matter how interesting and unusual it may be. In today’s ethology it is especially the traditional understanding of the herd and its hierarchy, the horse’s ability to learn, as well as friendships in groups of horses, that are the centre of attention, because many traditional ideas about horses have long since been disproven in these areas, as we shall see throughout the book.

 

A Brief Excursion into Evolutionary Biology

When we think of the term ‘evolution’, we typically think of the science of genetic relationships between species. The various phenotypes of animal and plant species have evolved continuously over millions of years. Explaining, among other things, the relationship between the horse’s behaviour and its environment, Charles Darwin’s and Alfred Russell Wallace’s theories on the origin of species are some of the most important aspects of evolutionary theory. Almost simultaneously, both proved impressively that the individuals of a population differ slightly from each other, and that small variations, a certain variability in the phenotype, are passed on to their descendants. Nowadays of course we know about the existence of genes as the carriers of inherited information. It is the genes that make the long evolutionary processes down to the horse, and indeed to us humans, unambiguously explicable.

Ever since the beginning of life on earth, more individuals were born of all species than their various habitats could support. Therefore, they had, and still have, to compete for the existing resources and the transmission of their own genes. Natural selection, i.e. the pressure that differing environmental circumstances impose on each individual, leads to a thinning of the population. Those who meet this selective pressure most successfully leave the most offspring behind, and transfer their genetic material into the future. A population changes over many generations in such a way that it retains only the traits of those individuals that were adapted best to their environment. In horses, their impressive speed and endurance can be seen as a selective advantage, because the slower specimens were more likely to fall victim topredators, and their genetic material disappeared for ever.

The sequence of the horse’s ancestry, which is very well documented through extensive fossil finds, is considered the epitome of evolutionary theory. The horse has evolved, over a period of approximately 60 million years, from the forest-dwelling and rather antelope-like eohippus to merychippus (a herd animal that lived in the steppe 20 to 25 million years ago), to pliohippus, which was structurally very similar to the horse(6 to 12 million years ago), and to our modern equus caballus.

It was only during the last 5000 years that humans have exercised a substantial influence

on the development of the horse. This process of keeping and breeding animals in captivity is called domestication. Humans selected specific traits among the original subspecies of the horse and bred different breeds, depending on the nature of the task for which they were used, or according to subjective ideas of beauty, by selectively breeding from particular parent animals, whose genes were then passed on to future generations.

 

The horse has always been a herd animal. Furthering the understanding of the complexities of hierarchy is one of the main tasks of today’s equine behavioural researchers.

 

Development of Behaviour and Personality

Development of Behaviour and Personality

I

n order to understand the personality of a horse, we first have to examine and understand the original aspects of its life that were not influenced by humans. Essential character traits are formed during the first days, weeks and months of its life. We should carefully consider at what time the contact between humans and the newborn foal should take place, and how intense it should be, so that the horse matures into a stable individual who trusts humans.